Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2017 Feb 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2015 Nov 26;55(2):122–129.e1. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2015.11.007

Early Childhood Behavioral Inhibition Predicts Cortical Thickness in Adulthood

Chad M Sylvester 1, Deanna M Barch 2, Michael P Harms 3, Andy C Belden 4, Timothy J Oakberg 5, Andrea L Gold 6, Lauren K White 7, Brenda E Benson 8, Sonya Troller-Renfree 9, Kathryn A Degnan 10, Heather A Henderson 11, Joan L Luby 12, Nathan A Fox 13, Daniel S Pine 14
PMCID: PMC4724382  NIHMSID: NIHMS741349  PMID: 26802779

Abstract

Objective

Behavioral inhibition (BI) during early childhood predicts risk for anxiety disorders and altered cognitive control in adolescence. Although BI has been linked to variation in brain function through adulthood, few studies have examined relations between early childhood BI and adult brain structure.

Method

The relation between early childhood BI and cortical thickness in adulthood was examined in a cohort of individuals followed since early childhood (N=53, mean age 20.5 years). Analyses tested whether anxiety and/or cognitive control during adolescence moderated relations between BI and cortical thickness. Cognitive control was measured with the Eriksen Flanker task. Initial analyses examined cortical thickness in regions of interest previously implicated in BI, anxiety disorders, and cognitive control: dorsal anterior cingulate (dACC), anterior insula (aI), and subgenual anterior cingulate (sgACC); and volumes of the amygdala and hippocampus. Exploratory analyses examined relations across the prefrontal cortex.

Results

BI during early childhood related to thinner dACC in adulthood. Neither anxiety nor cognitive control moderated this relation. A stronger congruency effect on the Flanker task during adolescence independently related to thinner dACC in adulthood. Higher anxiety during adolescence related to thicker cortex in the right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC) in adulthood among those with low BI as children.

Conclusion

Temperament in early childhood and the interaction between temperament and later anxiety relate to adult brain structure. These results are consistent with prior work associating BI and anxiety with functional brain variability in the dACC and VLPFC.

Keywords: Behavioral inhibition, anxiety, cortical thickness, structural MRI, cingulate

INTRODUCTION

Behavioral inhibition (BI) is a temperament defined by high negative reactivity to novelty, particularly in social contexts.1,2 Some toddlers high in BI 3,4 exhibit social reticence through the school years and face high risk for anxiety disorders through early adulthood.57 An early history of childhood BI also predicts patterns of brain function through adulthood,8,9 especially during tasks that measure cognitive control, such as the Eriksen Flanker Task.1012 Interestingly, prior research suggests that anxiety symptoms in adolescence moderate relations between atypical brain activity and BI.10,13,14 Although several studies link early childhood BI to variation in brain function during adolescence and adulthood, few studies examine the relations between early childhood BI and adult brain structure. The current study examines these relations.

Behavioral inhibition, anxiety disorders, and cognitive control have all been linked to functional variation in a common set of brain regions, including the amygdala, hippocampus, dorsal anterior cingulate (dACC), anterior insula (aI), and subgenual anterior cingulate (sgACC).1522 The amygdala and hippocampus, in the bilateral medial temporal lobes, play roles in evaluating the emotional significance of stimuli.23 Variation in functioning of these structures may relate to increased emotional reactivity to specific stimuli.24,25 The dACC and anterior insula in the prefrontal cortex both are members of a “cingulo-opercular” network putatively involved in adjusting cognitive control in response to errors.26 Activity in the dACC in response to errors appears to be increased in both BI and anxiety disorders, and variation in the function of the dACC may explain increased error sensitively associated with these phenotypes.10 Finally, the sgACC in the medial prefrontal cortex is thought to regulate amygdala activity, and variation in this region has been associated with poorer emotion regulation.27

While there are well-established links between early childhood BI and later brain function, little is known about the relation between BI and adult brain structure. In contrast to functional brain studies, measures of brain structure do not depend on the particular task chosen by the experimenter or moment-to-moment fluctuations in mood state. Thus, structural brain studies complement functional studies by elucidating enduring, state-independent structural correlates of early childhood BI. Early childhood BI could be associated with altered adult brain structure either because such differences are present at birth or because high BI relates to behaviors or experiences associated with atypical brain development. In either case, linking adult brain structure to early childhood BI biologically connects early childhood BI to an adult measure that could inform outcome prediction and early intervention in children with high BI.

Prior research provides initial evidence for a link between individual differences in BI and brain structure. Schwartz et al.28 used a longitudinal design and compared cortical thickness in young adults with or without histories of high reactivity at 4 months of age, an early marker of BI. Adults with high reactivity at 4 months of age had thinner left orbitofrontal cortex and thicker right ventromedial prefrontal cortex compared to adults with low reactivity at 4 months of age. Another study examining relations between brain structure during adulthood and retrospective report of BI during childhood noted increased amygdala and caudate volumes associated with BI.29 The current study examines adult brain structure in relation to a stable measure of BI acquired prospectively over 4 assessments from 14 months to 7 years of age. Given that prospectively acquired measures of stable BI more consistently relate to later-life psychopathology than measures collected at a single time point,5 the current study uses a phenotype relevant to functioning in adolescence and young adulthood.

The current study tests the hypothesis that BI measured during early childhood predicts cortical thickness measured during adulthood. This study further tests whether relations between early childhood BI and cortical thickness in adulthood are moderated by anxiety and/or altered cognitive control occurring during adolescence. Such moderation is expected, given that both of these conditions have been linked to BI and have been associated with an overlapping set of brain regions. To address these questions, the study utilizes a well-characterized, rigorously ascertained cohort containing individuals followed since 4 months of age through early adulthood.3,30 This unique longitudinal cohort permits a rare opportunity to test prospective relations. Initial analyses test whether childhood BI predicts adult cortical thickness in regions commonly associated with BI, anxiety, and cognitive control: dACC, aI, or sgACC; or volume of the amygdala or hippocampus. These analyses test whether anxiety and/or cognitive control measured at intervening time points (in adolescence) moderate any relations detected between early childhood BI and cortical thickness in adulthood. Parallel exploratory vertex-wise analyses examine these same relations across all of the prefrontal cortex.

METHOD

Participants

Participants were derived from an ongoing longitudinal study of BI in which behavioral data were available beginning at 4 months of age.3,30 Participants were originally recruited from the suburban Washington, DC area through commercially available mailing lists. Potential participants were screened over the telephone and excluded on the basis of prematurity, low birth weight, or perinatal complications. A home or lab visit was then scheduled for 433 infants (from 2 separate cohorts) within 2 weeks of their 4-month birthday, and infants were assessed for motor reactivity and emotional reaction to novel stimuli. Infants at the extreme ends of these measures were invited to participate in the full longitudinal study on the basis of selecting infants at high and low risk of developing behaviorally inhibited temperament.3,31 A small number of participants (5 in the sample used in the current study) were added to the study in early childhood to serve as unfamiliar peers in a social lab assessment (4 or 7 years of age).

A total of 163 were potential participants for the current study on the basis of having participated in BI assessments during childhood as well as anxiety assessments during adolescence or young adulthood. Of these 163 participants, 25 refused participation in the young adult study, and an additional 105 were either excluded or not approached on the basis of the following: contraindication to magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), psychotropic medication use, psychopathology requiring immediate clinical attention, recent completion of other studies, and/or participant request not to be contacted for a period of time after which the current study was completed.12 Acceptable neuroimaging data were therefore available for a subset of participants (n = 53, mean age 20.5 years, 29 male). Table 1 lists demographics for this subset and participation rates for the seven time points where data were acquired (ages 4 months; 14 months; 24 months; 4 years; 7 years; early adolescence, mean age 14.7 years; and young adulthood, mean age 19.8 years).

Table 1.

Demographic Characteristics and Assessment Participation Rates

Female 24 (45.3)

Age, y, m (SD) 20.5 (1.62)

Ethnicity,
 White 53 (100)

Maternal Education
 High school graduate 3 (5.7)
 Some college or technical school 6 (11.3)
 College graduate 26 (49.1)
 Graduate professional training 15 (28.3)
 Other/no information 3 (5.7)

IQ, m (SD) 114.1 (10.5)

Data Available for Assessment
 Age 4 months visit 43 (81.1)
 Age 14 months visita 44 (83.0)
 Age 24 months visita 45 (84.9)
 Age 4 years visita 45 (84.9)
 Age 7 years visita 44 (83.0)
 Adolescent visit (mean age 14.7 y)b 49 (92.5)
 Young adult visit (mean age 19.8 y)b 52 (98.1)
 Conflict task visit (mean age 14.8 y) 29 (54.7)

Note: Data are expressed as n (%) except where noted.

a

Indicates assessments that were used to compute the behavioral inhibition score.

b

Indicates assessments that were used to calculate the anxiety score.

Children were observed for an assessment of BI at 14 and 24 months of age and for an assessment of social reticence at 4 and 7 years of age.1,32,33 BI at 14 and 24 months was assessed by measuring infants’ reactions to novel objects and people.3 Mothers also reported their child’s social fear at 14 and 24 months using the Toddler Behavior Assessment Questionnaire.34 Social reticence is considered a marker of BI during the early school-age years. For the assessments of social reticence at ages 4 and 7 years, children’s reticent behavior with three unfamiliar peers was measured with Rubin’s Play Observation Scale.33 Mothers also rated their child’s social fear at 4 and 7 years using the shyness subscale of the Colorado Child Temperament Inventory.35 For each of the four time points, observed behavioral and maternal report measures were standardized in the full cohort and then averaged to create a single measure of early childhood BI.9,36,37 For our primary analyses, we used this continuous composite measure of early childhood BI and social reticence. This composite provides a stable measure of early childhood BI and has been used extensively in prior work.11,12,36,37 For a secondary analysis to compare results with other studies, we categorized participants on the basis of their reactivity profiles at 4 months of age. This analysis attempts to replicate findings from the only other longitudinal structural brain study of young adults with histories of BI in infancy.28 All 53 participants had the primary composite measure of BI available. For the secondary analysis, 43 had complete data for the 4-month reactivity phenotype available (of which 19 were classified as high negative reactive and 11 as low reactive).

In addition to examining the influence of early childhood BI, we wished to test whether anxiety and/or cognitive control during adolescence moderated the relation between early childhood BI and brain structure in young adulthood. This analysis minimized the number of statistical comparisons and derived a measure of anxiety incorporating as much data from the current study as possible. Accordingly, a single composite measure of anxiety over adolescence and young adulthood was created for each participant. This composite measure was created on the basis of three measures obtained during adolescence (mean age 14.7 years): parent report from Screen for Child Anxiety Related Disorders (SCARED), child report from SCARED,38 and the Anxious/Depressed raw score on the Youth Self-Report (YSR);39 and three measures obtained during young adulthood (mean age 19.8 years): Beck Anxiety Inventory,40 Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale,41 and the Anxious/Depressed raw score on the YSR. Standardized scores were created for each measure for each participant on the basis of all participants with available data on these anxiety measures (n = 163), not just the subset imaged for this current study. The use of standardized scores allows an equal weighting of each measure in the composite. The composite anxiety measure was an average over these six standardized measures (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.71). The adolescent and young adult composites were correlated (Spearman’s rho = 0.48, p<.001), suggesting trait-like features. Data were available in over 80% of the imaged participants for each individual scale, 92.5% of participants had at least one measure from adolescence, and 98.1% had at least one measure from adulthood. All participants had at least two of the six measures available.

The Eriksen Flanker task was used to measure cognitive control in the current sample. A subset of the cohort with imaging data in young adulthood had completed the Eriksen Flanker task in adolescence (n = 25, mean age 14.8 years, 9 female). The Flanker task consisted of equal numbers of congruent (HHHHH and SSSSS) and incongruent (HHSHH and SSHSS) trials in which participants had to indicate the central letter with a button press.42 Cognitive control was measured with the congruency effect, computed as the difference in reaction time for incongruent versus congruent trials, divided by the reaction time on congruent trials. Higher scores indicate poorer cognitive control.

Neuroimaging

All MRI data were acquired on the same scanner, a GE Healthcare MR750 3.0 Tesla scanner with a 32-channel head coil. Each scanning session included a single high-resolution, T1-weighted structural imaging sequence (MPRAGE; sagittal acquisition; 176 slices; 1 mm3 isotropic voxels; 256 × 256 matrix; flip angle = 7°; repetition time [TR] = 7.7 ms; echo time [TE] = 3.42 ms; inversion time [TI] = 425 ms).

Image Processing

FreeSurfer 5.3 (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/) was used to generate subcortical segmentations,43,44 cortical parcellations using the Destrieux et al. atlas,45 and pial and white matter surfaces from the T1-weighted images.4648 After the parcellations and surfaces were generated, they were visually reviewed by trained research assistants and, where necessary, manually edited and regenerated. Regional volumes and cortical thicknesses were obtained from the edited parcellations and surfaces.45,48

Regional a priori Analyses

Three bilateral regions of interest (ROIs) from the Destrieux et al. atlas45 were selected a priori for initial cortical thickness analyses: middle anterior part of the cingulate gyrus and sulcus (dACC), short insular gyrus (aI), and the subcallosal gyrus (sgACC). These ROIs are depicted in Figure 1A. Bilateral volumes of the amygdalae and hippocampi were examined in a priori analyses as well. In an effort to replicate Schwartz et al.,28 two additional ROIs were explored exclusively in the secondary analysis when examining the relations between four-month temperament and cortical thickness: left orbitofrontal and right ventromedial regions were hand-drawn on an average FreeSurfer volume based on Figures 1 and 2 from Schwartz et al.28 These regions were then projected to individual participants to generate thickness values for the left orbitofrontal and right ventromedial regions for each participant.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Early childhood behavioral inhibition predicts cortical thickness in the dorsal anterior cingulate (dACC) in young adulthood. Note: Panel A illustrates the 3 cortical a priori regions of interest, derived from the Destrieux atlas.45 Panel B depicts the significant relation between early childhood behavioral inhibition (BI) and cortical thickness in the dACC in adulthood. Each dot represents a single participant, and thickness values are averaged across left (L) and right (R) dACC. The encircled participant’s BI value was Winsorized because of BI > 3 SD from the mean. sgACC = subgenual anterior cingulate.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Anxiety during adolescence moderates the relation between early childhood behavioral inhibition (BI) and thickness of the right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC) during adulthood. Note: Panel A depicts a z-map for the interaction between early childhood BI and anxiety in adolescence in relation to cortical thickness in adulthood. Illustrated results survive cluster-wise multiple comparison correction across prefrontal cortex (p<.05). Panel B graphically illustrates the interaction for the right VLPFC region in Panel A. Higher anxiety during adolescence is related to increased thickness of the right VLPFC during adulthood among individuals with low but not high early childhood BI.

Prefrontal Cortex Exploratory Analyses

We additionally performed exploratory analyses in the prefrontal cortex.28 General linear modeling programs available through FreeSurfer (Qdec) were used for these vertex-wise analyses. A prefrontal cortex mask was generated by combining the following parcellations from the Desikan atlas:49 superior frontal, rostral, and caudal middle frontal, pars opercularis, pars triangularis, pars orbitalis, lateral and medial orbitofrontal, frontal pole, and rostral and caudal anterior cingulate cortices. To correct for multiple comparisons, Monte Carlo simulations available in the FreeSurfer software were used to determine that an area of cortex 68.1 mm2 with each individual vertex p<.001 was required to meet prefrontal cortex-wide cluster-wise significance of p<.05. Clusters meeting multiple comparison correction are reported in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates.

Statistical Analyses

All statistical tests were performed using SPSS version 20 (Armonk, NY) with the exception of the prefrontal cortex vertex-wise analyses, which were carried out using FreeSurfer. T-tests were used to compare BI and anxiety in the subset of participants that provided versus did not provide imaging data; chi-square tests were used to test for potential group differences in gender. Analogous tests compared participants in the current study that did versus did not provide Flanker task data in adolescence.

The relations among temperament, anxiety, and cortical thickness were examined with repeated measures analyses of variance (ANOVAs) predicting thickness. Specifically, for each a priori region, cortical thickness values from left and right hemispheres were included in a single model, with hemisphere treated as a repeated measure. Additional factors for the primary models comprised childhood BI, anxiety in adolescence/young adulthood, the interaction between BI and anxiety, gender, and whole-brain average cortical thickness. Variables were mean centered before computing interactions for all analyses. When significant effects emerged in primary models, secondary models added IQ and maternal education as covariates. Three participants each were missing IQ and maternal education data, and these missing values were replaced with group means; excluding these participants did not change the significance of results. Only results from secondary models are reported; primary models were included to ensure that results were not driven exclusively because of the addition of multiple covariates. For amygdala and hippocampus analyses, volumes were used instead of thicknesses, and whole-brain volume was used as a covariate instead of whole-brain average thickness. Bonferroni correction was used to protect against false positives for the five models based on the a priori regions.

Prefrontal cortex vertex-wise analyses were conducted separately for each hemisphere using the primary model with FreeSurfer. One participant’s composite BI measure was three standard deviations from the mean and another participant’s composite anxiety measure was three standard deviations from the mean. These outliers were handled by Winsorizing for all analyses; no outliers were detected in other measures.

Analyses examining the relations between cognitive control and cortical thickness of dACC were based on Westlye et al.50 The congruency effect on the Eriksen Flanker task for each participant was computed as the difference in reaction time for incongruent versus congruent trials divided by the reaction time on congruent trials. Higher scores indicate poorer cognitive control, and the mean score across the sample was 0.10 with a standard deviation of 0.051. To attempt replication, the relation between congruency and cortical thickness was examined with a repeated measure ANOVA with hemisphere as a repeated measure and congruency, gender, and IQ as covariates. This model was followed up by additionally controlling for whole-hemisphere thickness. Finally, we included congruency, early childhood BI, and the interaction between BI and congruency in a single model to determine whether congruency moderated the relation between BI and cortical thickness.

Power analyses were computed using G*Power 3.1.51 Using a single predictor in a regression model to detect a moderate effect size (partial η2=0.15), power in the full sample of 53 was 0.84, power in the sample using just the 4-month phenotype (n=30) was 0.59, and power in the sample using the Flanker data (n=25) was 0.51. Power to detect a larger effect size (partial η2=0.25, on the order of the effect size of the main result in this study) was 0.98 in the full sample, 0.86 for the 4-month phenotype data, and 0.79 for the Flanker data.

RESULTS

Participants

Compared to participants from the larger longitudinal study that were not imaged, participants that were imaged for the current study had significantly lower anxiety scores over adolescence/young adulthood (−0.18, SD 0.57 vs. 0.16, SD 0.91, t=2.5, p=.012, d=0.45), but there were no differences in early childhood BI scores (−0.06, SD 0.73 vs. 0.012, SD 0.60, t=0.71, p=.48, d=0.11) or gender (χ2 = 1.5, p=.22). Follow-up analyses revealed that imaged participants had significantly lower levels of anxiety compared to excluded participants (−0.18 vs. 0.17, t=2.6, p=.012). There was no difference in anxiety scores between the imaged participants and participants that refused participation (−0.18 vs. −0.07, t=0.7, p=.49). Note, however, that the absolute difference in anxiety between imaged participants and the total cohort was less than 0.2 SDs from the cohort mean, and the imaged participants were representative of the original longitudinal cohort in terms of BI scores. Within the current study sample, there were no significant differences in anxiety, BI, or gender (t=1.39, p=.17, d=0.38; t=0.24, p=.81, d=0.07; χ2 = 0.23, p=.63, respectively) between the subset that participated versus the subset that did not participate in the Flanker task in adolescence. Early childhood BI and anxiety in adolescence/young adulthood were not significantly related in the sample used in this study (r= −0.043, p=.76).

Behavioral Inhibition, Anxiety, and Cortical Thickness: Regional Analysis

The relations among BI during early childhood, anxiety in adolescence/young adulthood, and cortical thickness in young adulthood were examined in three bilateral a priori cortical regions of interest: the dorsal anterior cingulate (dACC), anterior insula (aI), and subgenual anterior cingulate (sgACC). These regions are depicted in Figure 1A. Parallel analyses examined relations with hippocampus and amygdala volumes. As depicted in Figure 1B, early childhood BI predicted thinner cortex in the dACC with a large effect size (F[1,45] = 16.2, p<.001 uncorrected, partial η2=0.26, survived Bonferroni correction for five a priori regions). This analysis controlled for multiple potential confounding factors, including gender, maternal education, IQ, whole-brain average cortical thickness, anxiety in adolescence/young adulthood, and the interaction between BI and anxiety. Of note, similar results emerged in bivariate analyses examining only the relations between BI and dACC thickness. Early childhood BI similarly predicted thinner cortex in the sgACC (F[1,45] = 5.2, p=.027 uncorrected, partial η2=0.10), although with a smaller effect size. Unlike findings for the dACC, this result did not survive Bonferroni correction for five comparisons. Neither anxiety nor the interaction between BI and anxiety was significantly related to dACC or sgACC cortical thickness, suggesting that anxiety did not moderate the relation between early childhood BI and thickness of the dACC in adulthood. There were no significant relations among childhood BI, anxiety in adolescence/young adulthood, or the interaction between BI and anxiety with anterior insula thickness or hippocampus or amygdala volumes.

Behavioral Inhibition, Anxiety, and Cortical Thickness: Prefrontal Cortex Exploratory Analysis

To complement the a priori ROI analysis, a parallel analysis examined the relations among BI during early childhood, anxiety in adolescence/young adulthood, and cortical thickness in young adulthood in an exploratory fashion across prefrontal cortex. In contrast to the regional analysis, the prefrontal cortex vertex-wise analysis did not detect any regions surviving multiple comparison correction that were related to early childhood BI alone. A whole-brain vertex-wise map, uncorrected at p<.05, demonstrating the relation between early childhood BI and cortical thickness in young adulthood, is provided in Figure S1, available online. This map is consistent with the regional analysis, as the thickness of a large swath of cortex near the dACC was related to early childhood BI; as above, however, this cluster did not survive multiple comparison correction across prefrontal cortex. The prefrontal cortex vertex-wise analysis likewise did not detect any regions surviving multiple-comparison correction that were related to anxiety in adolescence/young adulthood.

The prefrontal cortex vertex-wise analysis did, however, detect a region in which thickness in young adulthood was related to the interaction between early childhood BI and anxiety in adolescence/young adulthood. As illustrated in Figure 2, cortical thickness in an 83 mm2 patch of the right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC; centered at +48.2 +10.0 +13.9 in MNI coordinates, within the pars triangularis) was predicted by an interaction between childhood BI and anxiety in adolescence/young adulthood, controlling for gender and whole-hemisphere thickness (p=.024, corrected for multiple comparisons across prefrontal cortex). To explore the source of this interaction, a median split divided participants into those with low versus high levels of early childhood BI, and a similar median split was performed for anxiety. Note that effect sizes for this median split analysis are biased by the circular nature of the analysis, which computes statistics on a region initially identified in an exploratory test. Effect sizes are reported solely to interpret the interaction. In participants with low early childhood BI, a high level of anxiety during adolescence/young adulthood predicted thicker VLPFC compared to a low level of anxiety (d=1.56). In participants with high early childhood BI, however, anxiety during adolescence/young adulthood was unrelated to VLPFC thickness in young adulthood (d= −0.46).

Temperament at Four Months

The characterization of early childhood BI discussed above was a composite of measures from age 14 months to 7 years. Analyses in the only other study using a similar design, Schwartz et al.,28 relied on data from 4 months of age. In secondary analyses, we used phenotypes similar to this prior study in the small subset (57%) of our participants with either high reactive negative (n = 19) or low reactive (n = 11) temperament at age 4 months. This measure did not predict cortical thickness in young adulthood in any of the a priori regions tested including the left orbitofrontal and right ventromedial regions derived from Schwartz et al.28 Prefrontal cortex vertex-wise analyses similarly did not reveal any significant relations between 4-month temperament and regional cortical thickness. Using a less stringent threshold of p<.05 uncorrected also did not reveal any relations between cortical thickness near the regions derived from Schwartz et al.28 and temperament at 4 months.

Cognitive Control

We examined whether congruency effects on the Eriksen Flanker task during adolescence moderated the relations between early childhood BI and dACC thickness in young adulthood. Higher congruency effects on the Eriksen Flanker task during adolescence (reflecting poorer cognitive control) predicted thinner dACC during young adulthood (F[1,23] = 4.66, p=.042, partial η2=0.17), controlling for gender and IQ. There was no relation, however, between BI in early childhood and the congruency effect in young adulthood among the current study sample (r=−0.01, p=.94). The interaction between BI and congruency likewise was unrelated to dACC thickness (F[1,21] = 2.12, p=.16, partial η2=0.09), indicating that the congruency effect (one measure of cognitive control) did not moderate the relation between early childhood BI and young adult dACC thickness.

DISCUSSION

The primary goal of the current study was to test the hypothesis that BI in early childhood predicts thickness in specific cortical regions, as well as volume of amygdala and hippocampus, during adulthood. Consistent with this hypothesis, BI during early childhood predicted thinner dACC in adulthood, although no differences as a function of BI were detected in anterior insula, sgACC (after correction for multiple comparisons), amygdala, or hippocampus. Neither anxiety nor cognitive control during adolescence moderated the relation between early childhood BI and dACC thickness in adulthood, although a higher congruency effect on the Eriksen Flanker task during adolescence was independently related to thinner dACC in adulthood. High anxiety during adolescence and young adulthood was related to thicker cortex in the right VLPFC in young adulthood, but only among those who had low BI as children.

These results are consistent with prior literature linking BI to the dACC. The dACC is part of a network of brain regions involved in identifying and signaling the need for increased cognitive control.26 A series of studies in the current and other samples use event-related potentials42,52,53 and functional MRI (fMRI)11,12 to measure brain activity during tasks that require trial-by-trial changes in levels of cognitive control. This series of studies demonstrates consistently larger increases in neuronal activity in or near the dACC among children and adolescents with high BI relative to those with low BI.

These data are consistent with a model in which the dACC of individuals high in BI is highly sensitive to conflict, signaling the need for cognitive control.10 Notably, research has shown this BI-related pattern of brain function does not generate behavioral benefits but rather relates to risk for anxiety.10 In light of this model, the current findings of thinner dACC among individuals with higher BI suggests the hypotheses that either a thinner dACC generates a larger neural signal or that repeatedly high activity in the dACC over development results in thinner cortex. Longitudinal studies incorporating neuroimaging early in life are needed to adjudicate these possibilities. In either case, these data may provide a biological explanation for the link between early childhood BI and adult outcomes. Moreover, by linking early temperament to adult brain structure, the current findings raise questions about the types of impairing behaviors expressed in adulthood that could relate to both early-childhood temperament and dACC structure. For example, adults with a childhood history of high BI may have subtle abnormalities of cognitive control associated with decreased dACC thickness. If so, interventions targeting impairment in children with high BI could influence these or other behaviors in adulthood.

Importantly, variation in neither adolescent anxiety nor cognitive control on the Eriksen Flanker task moderated the relation between early childhood BI and adult dACC thickness. These results suggest that the association between early childhood BI and adult dACC thickness manifests independently of anxiety and/or this one particular measure of cognitive control. One possibility is that although thinner dACC in individuals with a history of BI may increase risk for pathological anxiety and alterations in cognitive control, some individuals compensate for this risk. Further studies could test whether this compensation occurs functionally within the dACC (i.e. activity is normal even though structure is abnormal) or through functional and/or structural changes in other brain areas.

In the current study, adolescent/young adult anxiety moderated the relations between early childhood BI and cortical thickness in a portion of the right VLPFC. High anxiety in adolescence and adulthood related to increased VLPFC thickness in young adults with a history of low BI in early childhood. No such relation occurred in adults with high BI in early childhood. These results suggest that the neurodevelopmental pathway to high anxiety could differ depending on underlying temperament,10 with variation in the VLPFC related to anxiety only in individuals low in BI. Previous work consistently relates variation in the structure and function of the right VLFPC to both anxiety disorders and orienting to threat.5456 The right VLPFC may be a portion of the ventral attention network, involved in stimulus-driven attention, and increased thickness may be related to increased orienting to threat and subsequently anxiety.27,57,58 Because VLPFC thickness is only relevant to individuals low in BI during early childhood, a speculative possibility is that the mechanisms of orienting to threat and the pathway to anxiety differ depending on levels of BI.

In a secondary analysis that was only able to use a small percentage (57%) of our participants, we did not replicate Schwartz et al.,28 who reported thinner left orbitofrontal cortex and thicker right ventromedial prefrontal cortex in young adults with a history of high reactivity as 4-month-old infants. This difference could reflect low power associated with small sample size, as well as methodological factors. Nevertheless, the current study, Schwartz et al.,28 and other prior work7 suggest that early childhood BI predicts aspects of brain structure or function later in adulthood. Moreover, additional analyses replicated prior work linking dACC thickness to congruency effects on the Eriksen Flanker task.50,59 Thus, the current findings replicate some brain–behavior associations seen in prior work.

The results of this study should be interpreted in light of several limitations. We had a moderate sample size for the primary analysis and a small sample for our secondary analyses. As a result, failure to detect associations could reflect a Type II error. Small sample sizes reflect the difficulty of acquiring brain imaging data on participants followed prospectively for over 20 years. Demonstrating large effect sizes in some analyses, however, speaks to the value of such work, even with limited sample size. Another consideration is that the sample used in this study had a small (less than 0.2 SDs) but significant difference in the amount of anxiety experienced during adolescence and young adulthood relative to the whole longitudinal cohort.

In summary, the current study demonstrates that variation in early childhood temperament is related to adult brain structure. These data reinforce the hypothesis that the dACC is a key brain structure in the physiology of BI and provide a candidate biological basis for the associations between early childhood BI and adult functional outcomes. Future studies following cohorts incorporating longitudinal neuroimaging that begins earlier in childhood are needed to determine whether alterations are present early in childhood or emerge later in the course of development. In addition, future studies are required to determine the functional implications of the cortical thickness differences detected in this study. Regardless of the outcome of these additional studies, data from the current study highlight the importance of early intervention for children that are functionally impaired from high BI because of the potential for enduring effects on brain structure that occur into adulthood.

Supplementary Material

suppl. Figure S1.

Whole-brain, vertex-wise map demonstrating the relation between early childhood behavioral inhibition (BI) and cortical thickness in young adulthood. Note: Results are adjusted for anxiety during adolescence/young adulthood, the interaction between BI and anxiety, sex, and whole-hemisphere mean cortical thickness. Maps are thresholded at p<.05, uncorrected. Warmer colors indicate a positive relation between BI and cortical thickness, and cooler colors indicate a negative relation. Although no patches of cortex were significant after correcting for multiple comparisons across prefrontal cortex, the highest peak in these maps is centered near the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, consistent with the regional analyses.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by National Institutes of Health grants T32MH100019 (C.S.), R01MH090786 (D.B. and J.L.), R37HD017899 (N.F.), U01MH093349 (N.F.), R01MH074454 (N.F.); the Parker Fund (C.S.); the Taylor Institute (C.S.); and the NIMH Intramural Research Program (D.P.).

Footnotes

Supplementary material cited in this article is available online.

Dr. Degnan served as the statistical expert for this research.

Disclosure: Dr. Barch has served on an advisory board for Takeda, has a contract with Pfizer, and has consulted for Amgen and Pfizer. All of these roles are related to schizophrenia research. Dr. Luby has received royalties from Guilford Press. Drs. Sylvester, Harms, Belden, Gold, White, Benson, Troller-Renfree, Degnan, Henderson, Fox, and Pine, and Mr. Oakberg report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.

This article is discussed in an editorial by Dr. Matthew Albaugh on page xx.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Contributor Information

Dr. Chad M. Sylvester, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis.

Dr. Deanna M. Barch, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis.

Dr. Michael P. Harms, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis.

Dr. Andy C. Belden, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis.

Mr. Timothy J. Oakberg, University of Colorado Denver.

Dr. Andrea L. Gold, The National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) Intramural Research Program, Bethesda, MD.

Dr. Lauren K. White, The National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) Intramural Research Program, Bethesda, MD.

Dr. Brenda E. Benson, The National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) Intramural Research Program, Bethesda, MD.

Dr. Sonya Troller-Renfree, University of Maryland, College Park.

Dr. Kathryn A. Degnan, University of Maryland, College Park.

Dr. Heather A. Henderson, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, ON, Canada.

Dr. Joan L. Luby, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis.

Dr. Nathan A. Fox, University of Maryland, College Park.

Dr. Daniel S. Pine, The National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) Intramural Research Program, Bethesda, MD.

References

  • 1.Fox NA, Henderson HA, Marshall PJ, Nichols KE, Ghera MM. Behavioral inhibition: Linking biology and behavior within a developmental framework. Ann Rev Psychology. 2005;56:235–62. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Kagan J, Reznick JS, Snidman N, Gibbons J, Johnson MO. Childhood derivatives of inhibition and lack of inhibition to the unfamiliar. Child Dev. 1988;59:1580–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1988.tb03685.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Fox NA, Henderson HA, Rubin KH, Calkins SD, Schmidt LA. Continuity and discontinuity of behavioral inhibition and exuberance: psychophysiological and behavioral influences across the first four years of life. Child Development. 2001;72:1–21. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Gest SD. Behavioral inhibition: stability and associations with adaptation from childhood to early adulthood. J Personality Social Psychology. 1997;72:467–75. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.72.2.467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Chronis-Tuscano A, Degnan KA, Pine DS, et al. Stable early maternal report of behavioral inhibition predicts lifetime social anxiety disorder in adolescence. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48:928–35. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181ae09df. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Hirshfeld DR, Rosenbaum JF, Biederman J, et al. Stable behavioral inhibition and its association with anxiety disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1992;31:103–11. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199201000-00016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Clauss JA, Blackford JU. Behavioral inhibition and risk for developing social anxiety disorder: a meta-analytic study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;51:1066–1075. e1061. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.08.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Schwartz CE, Wright CI, Shin LM, Kagan J, Rauch SL. Inhibited and uninhibited infants “grown up”: adult amygdalar response to novelty. Science. 2003;300:1952–3. doi: 10.1126/science.1083703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Perez-Edgar K, Roberson-Nay R, Hardin MG, et al. Attention alters neural responses to evocative faces in behaviorally inhibited adolescents. Neuroimage. 2007;35:1538–46. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Henderson HA, Pine DS, Fox NA. Behavioral inhibition and developmental risk: a dual-processing perspective. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40:207–24. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Jarcho JM, Fox NA, Pine DS, et al. Enduring influence of early temperament on neural mechanisms mediating attention-emotion conflict in adults. Depress Anxiety. 2014;31:53–62. doi: 10.1002/da.22140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Jarcho JM, Fox NA, Pine DS, et al. The neural correlates of emotion-based cognitive control in adults with early childhood behavioral inhibition. Biol Psychol. 2013;92:306–14. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.09.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Reeb-Sutherland BC, Vanderwert RE, Degnan KA, et al. Attention to novelty in behaviorally inhibited adolescents moderates risk for anxiety. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2009;50:1365–72. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02170.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Hardee JE, Benson BE, Bar-Haim Y, et al. Patterns of neural connectivity during an attention bias task moderate associations between early childhood temperament and internalizing symptoms in young adulthood. Biol Psychiatry. 2013;74:273–9. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.01.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Blackford JU, Pine DS. Neural substrates of childhood anxiety disorders: a review of neuroimaging findings. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 2012;21:501–25. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2012.05.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Strawn JR, Dominick KC, Patino LR, Doyle CD, Picard LS, Phan KL. Neurobiology of Pediatric Anxiety Disorders. Current behavioral neuroscience reports. 2014;1:154–160. doi: 10.1007/s40473-014-0014-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Shin LM, Liberzon I. The neurocircuitry of fear, stress, and anxiety disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:169–91. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Strawn JR, Hamm L, Fitzgerald DA, Fitzgerald KD, Monk CS, Phan KL. Neurostructural abnormalities in pediatric anxiety disorders. J Anxiety Disord. 2015;32:81–8. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2015.03.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Strawn JR, John Wegman C, Dominick KC, et al. Cortical surface anatomy in pediatric patients with generalized anxiety disorder. J Anxiety Disord. 2014;28:717–23. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2014.07.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Strawn JR, Wehry AM, Chu WJ, et al. Neuroanatomic abnormalities in adolescents with generalized anxiety disorder: a voxel-based morphometry study. Depress Anxiety. 2013;30:842–8. doi: 10.1002/da.22089. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Mueller SC, Aouidad A, Gorodetsky E, Goldman D, Pine DS, Ernst M. Gray matter volume in adolescent anxiety: an impact of the brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val(66)Met polymorphism? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2013;52:184–95. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.11.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Milham MP, Nugent AC, Drevets WC, et al. Selective reduction in amygdala volume in pediatric anxiety disorders: a voxel-based morphometry investigation. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57:961–6. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.01.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.LeDoux JE. Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual review of neuroscience. 2000;23:155–84. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Thomas KM, Drevets WC, Dahl RE, et al. Amygdala response to fearful faces in anxious and depressed children. Arch gen psychiatry. 2001;58:1057–63. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.11.1057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Monk CS, Telzer EH, Mogg K, et al. Amygdala and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex activation to masked angry faces in children and adolescents with generalized anxiety disorder. Arch gen psychiatry. 2008;65:568–76. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.65.5.568. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Dosenbach NUF, Fair DA, Cohen AL, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. A dual-networks architecture of top-down control. Trends in cognitive sciences. 2008;12:99–105. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.01.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Sylvester CM, Corbetta M, Raichle ME, et al. Functional network dysfunction in anxiety and anxiety disorders. Trends Neurosci. 2012;35:527–35. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2012.04.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Schwartz CE, Kunwar PS, Greve DN, et al. Structural differences in adult orbital and ventromedial prefrontal cortex predicted by infant temperament at 4 months of age. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67:78–84. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.171. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Clauss JA, Seay AL, VanDerKlok RM, et al. Structural and functional bases of inhibited temperament. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience. 2014;9:2049–58. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Fox NA, Rubin KH, Calkins SD, et al. Frontal activation asymmetry and social competence at four years of age. Child Dev. 1995;66:1770–84. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Calkins SD, Fox NA, Marshall TR. Behavioral and physiological antecedents of inhibited and uninhibited behavior. Child Dev. 1996;67:523–40. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Coplan RJ, Rubin KH, Fox NA, Calkins SD, Stewart SL. Being alone, playing alone, and acting alone: distinguishing among reticence and passive and active solitude in young children. Child Dev. 1994;65:129–37. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Rubin KH. The Play Observation Scale (POS) Waterloo, ON: University of Waterloo; 1989. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Goldsmith HH. Studying temperament via construction of the Toddler Behavior Assessment Questionnaire. Child Dev. 1996;67:218–35. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Rowe DC, Plomin R. Temperament in early childhood. J Personality assessment. 1977;41:150–6. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4102_5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Guyer AE, Benson B, Choate VR, et al. Lasting associations between early-childhood temperament and late-adolescent reward-circuitry response to peer feedback. Dev Psychopathol. 2014;26:229–43. doi: 10.1017/S0954579413000941. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Guyer AE, Jarcho JM, Perez-Edgar K, et al. Temperament and Parenting Styles in Early Childhood Differentially Influence Neural Response to Peer Evaluation in Adolescence. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2015;43:863–74. doi: 10.1007/s10802-015-9973-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Birmaher B, Brent DA, Chiappetta L, Bridge J, Monga S, Baugher M. Psychometric properties of the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED): a replication study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1999;38:1230–6. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199910000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Achenbach T. Manual for the Youth Self-Report and 1991 Profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Beck AT, Steer RA. Beck Anxiety Inventory Manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Liebowitz MR. Social phobia. Modern problems of pharmacopsychiatry. 1987;22:141–173. doi: 10.1159/000414022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Mcdermott JM, Perez-Edgar K, Henderson HA, Chronis-Tuscano A, Pine DS, Fox NA. A History of Childhood Behavioral Inhibition and Enhanced Response Monitoring in Adolescence Are Linked to Clinical Anxiety. Biological Psychiatry. 2009;65:445–8. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.10.043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Fischl B, Salat DH, Busa E, et al. Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron. 2002;33:341–55. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00569-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Fischl B, van der Kouwe A, Destrieux C, et al. Automatically parcellating the human cerebral cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2004;14:11–22. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhg087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Destrieux C, Fischl B, Dale A, Halgren E. Automatic parcellation of human cortical gyri and sulci using standard anatomical nomenclature. Neuroimage. 2010;53:1–15. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Dale AM, Fischl B, Sereno MI. Cortical surface-based analysis. I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage. 1999;9:179–94. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Dale AM, Sereno MI. Improved Localizadon of Cortical Activity by Combining EEG and MEG with MRI Cortical Surface Reconstruction: A Linear Approach. J Cogn Neurosci. 1993;5:162–76. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1993.5.2.162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Fischl B, Dale AM. Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2000;97:11050–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.200033797. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Desikan RS, Segonne F, Fischl B, et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage. 2006;31:968–980. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Westlye LT, Grydeland H, Walhovd KB, Fjell AM. Associations between regional cortical thickness and attentional networks as measured by the attention network test. Cereb Cortex. 2011;21:345–56. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang AG. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior research methods. 2009;41:1149–60. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Lahat A, Lamm C, Chronis-Tuscano A, Pine DS, Henderson HA, Fox NA. Early behavioral inhibition and increased error monitoring predict later social phobia symptoms in childhood. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2014;53:447–55. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.12.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Lamm C, Walker OL, Degnan KA, et al. Cognitive control moderates early childhood temperament in predicting social behavior in 7-year-old children: an ERP study. Dev Sci. 2014;17:667–81. doi: 10.1111/desc.12158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Pine DS, Fox NA. Childhood antecedents and risk for adult mental disorders. Annu Rev Psychol. 2015;66:459–85. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.McClure EB, Monk CS, Nelson EE, et al. Abnormal attention modulation of fear circuit function in pediatric generalized anxiety disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64:97–106. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.1.97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Britton JC, Bar-Haim Y, Carver FW, et al. Isolating neural components of threat bias in pediatric anxiety. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2012;53:678–86. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02503.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Sylvester CM, Barch DM, Corbetta M, Power JD, Schlaggar BL, Luby JL. Resting state functional connectivity of the ventral attention network in children with a history of depression or anxiety. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2013;52:1326–1336. e1325. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.10.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Sylvester CM, Hudziak JJ, Gaffrey MS, Barch DM, Luby JL. Stimulus-Driven Attention, Threat Bias, and Sad Bias in Youth with a History of an Anxiety Disorder or Depression [published online ahead of print Feb 2015] J Abnorm Child Psychol. doi: 10.1007/s10802-015-9988-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Fjell AM, Walhovd KB, Brown TT, et al. Multimodal imaging of the self-regulating developing brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012;109:19620–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1208243109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

suppl. Figure S1.

Whole-brain, vertex-wise map demonstrating the relation between early childhood behavioral inhibition (BI) and cortical thickness in young adulthood. Note: Results are adjusted for anxiety during adolescence/young adulthood, the interaction between BI and anxiety, sex, and whole-hemisphere mean cortical thickness. Maps are thresholded at p<.05, uncorrected. Warmer colors indicate a positive relation between BI and cortical thickness, and cooler colors indicate a negative relation. Although no patches of cortex were significant after correcting for multiple comparisons across prefrontal cortex, the highest peak in these maps is centered near the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, consistent with the regional analyses.

RESOURCES